Log In

Reset Password
BERMUDA | RSS PODCAST

If you’re smart, it doesn’t necessarily mean you’ll be rich

Brain power: research suggests intelligence has only a 1 or 2 per cent influence on how much you earn

How much is a child’s future success determined by innate intelligence? Economist James Heckman says it is not what people think. He likes to ask educated non-scientists — especially politicians and policymakers — how much of the difference between people’s incomes can be tied to IQ. Most guess about 25 per cent, even 50 per cent, he says. But the data suggests a much smaller influence: about 1 per cent or 2 per cent.

So if IQ is only a minor factor in success, what is it that separates the low earners from the high ones? Or, as the saying goes: If you’re so smart, why aren’t you rich?

Science does not have a definitive answer, although luck certainly plays a role. But another key factor is personality, according to a paper that Heckman co-wrote in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences last month. He found financial success was correlated with conscientiousness, a personality trait marked by diligence, perseverance and self-discipline.

To reach that conclusion, he and colleagues examined four different data sets, which, between them, included IQ scores, standardised test results, grades and personality assessments for thousands of people in Britain, the United States and the Netherlands. Some of the data sets followed people over decades, tracking not only income but criminal records, body mass index and self-reported life satisfaction.

The study found that grades and achievement-test results were markedly better predictors of adult success than raw IQ scores. That may seem surprising — after all, don’t they all measure the same thing? Not quite. Grades reflect not only intelligence but also what Heckman calls “non-cognitive skills”, such as perseverance, good study habits and the ability to collaborate — in other words, conscientiousness. To a lesser extent, the same is true of test scores. Personality counts.

Heckman, who shared a Nobel Prize in 2000 and is founder of the University of Chicago’s Centre for the Economics of Human Development, believes success hinges not only on innate ability but on skills that can be taught. His own research suggests childhood interventions can be helpful and that conscientiousness is more malleable than IQ. Openness, a broad trait that includes curiosity, is also connected to test scores and grades.

IQ still matters, of course. Someone with an IQ of 70 is not going to be able to do things that are easy for a person with an IQ of 190. But Heckman says many people fail to break into the job market because they lack skills that are not measured on intelligence tests. They don’t understand how to behave with courtesy in job interviews. They may show up late or fail to dress properly. Or on the job, they make it obvious they will do no more than the minimum, if that.

John Eric Humphries, a co-author of the paper, says he hoped their work could help to clarify the complicated, often misunderstood notion of ability. Even IQ tests, which were designed to assess innate problem-solving capabilities, appear to measure more than just smarts. In a 2011 study, University of Pennsylvania psychologist Angela Duckworth found that IQ scores also reflected test-takers’ motivation and effort. Diligent, motivated children will work harder to answer tough questions than equally intelligent, but lazier ones.

Teaching personality or character traits in school would not be easy. For one thing, it is not always clear whether more of a trait is always better. The higher the better for IQ, and perhaps for conscientiousness as well. But personality researchers have suggested the middle ground is best for other traits — you do not want to be so introverted that you cannot speak up, or so extroverted that you cannot shut up and listen.

What does any of this have to do with economics? “Our ultimate goal is to improve human wellbeing,” Heckman says, and a significant determinant of wellbeing comes down to skills.

A newer study published this month in the journal Nature Human Behaviour focused on the flip side of success: hardship. After following 1,000 New Zealanders for more than 30 years, researchers concluded that tests of language, behavioural skills and cognitive abilities taken when children were only 3 years old could predict who was most likely to need welfare, commit crimes or become chronically ill.

The lead author of that paper, Duke University psychologist Terrie Moffitt, says she hopes the results will foster compassion and help, not stigma. Her results also suggested that helping people to improve certain kinds of skills before they are out of diapers would benefit everyone.

Faye Flam is a Bloomberg View columnist. She was a staff writer for Science magazine and a columnist for the Philadelphia Inquirer, and she is the author ofThe Score: How the Quest for Sex Has Shaped the Modern Man